Gowin's Vee (Novak and Gowin, 1984, see Section @Ref The significance and importance of research is not a
function of the degree of statistical significance reported in
the findings (as in quantitative studies) nor the clarity and
insightfulness of ethnography (as in qualitative studies). The
significance and importance of educational research derives from
a variety of factors related to both its context and its
findings. These factors reflect timeliness, innovation, demand,
and appropriateness, as well as more conventional parameters such
as accuracy, reliability, and validity.
The education and computing context in which this research was
carried out is one that reveals increased interest in cooperative
learning as an instructional strategy@Cite(AjoseSA90a). In
particular, cooperative learning environments may provide one of
the most appropriate contexts for the application of
computers in education@Cite(JohnsonDW86a). In conjunction with the
recent proliferation of applications of telecommunications
in the schools, it becomes
apparent that student-student interaction via computer is an emerging
area of importance for research and practice in education. Further,
as the field of
computer supported cooperative work (CSCW) grows, the implications
and opportunities for computer supported cooperative learning (CSCL)
become increasingly clear@Cite(DaviesD89a).
Computer-mediated communication (CMC) is a primary component of
both CSCW and CSCL. The importance of CMC is demonstrated by the
fact that even in its most rudimentary form as asynchronous,
text-based computer conferencing (CC), it has made a significant
impact on theorists from sociology@Cite(HiltzSR78a), computer
science@Cite(TuroffM89a), philosophy@Cite(FeenbergA87a),
education@Cite(HarasimLM90a), and communication
studies@Cite(LevinsonP86b). In this group, CC has been construed
as a major socio-technological force leading
to significant changes in human interaction. With somewhat less
stature, synchronous text-based CMC has also become worthy of formal
academic investigation. This is revealed through the work of
researchers in social
psychology@Cite(KieslerS84a), nursing education@Cite(SleightBJ89a),
and written communication@Cite(MurrayDE91a).
To round out the contextual legitimacy of the present study, it
is the needs of the nursing profession for the provision of
innovative preservice and inservice education to a geographically
dispersed target population that makes the developments in
educational CMC particularly appropriate. As well, the very
nature of CMC as a form of interpersonal verbal communication is
coincidental with interpersonal communication skill requirements
for the learning and practice of nursing.
The specific problem context for the present study is discussed
in Section @Ref(secprob) and will be briefly restated here. Since
it has been proposed that CMC be used
for educational delivery and student interaction, our attention is
drawn to those aspects of the medium, and the use of the medium,
that may affect the learning process. In cooperative learning,
verbal interaction among peers is the fundamental agent through
which cognitive activity transpires. Interpersonal cognitive
facilitation (ICF) may arise as an essential link between verbal
behaviour in peer discussions and the social construction of knowledge.
Peer discussions are normally face-to-face and synchronous, but
CMC affords the possibility of asynchronous (store and forward)
interaction. Proponents of asynchronous CMC claim that the
asynchronous nature of these interactions is the characteristic
most responsible for the intellectual benefits they report.
These theories and claims direct our focus to questions
about the effects of synchronicity on peer interactions in
educational CMC. Also, previous research concerning the verbal
behaviour of members of cooperative learning groups leads us to
methods of research involving the qualitative analysis of
communication content and appropriate mechanisms for
transformation of raw data to useful information. The primary
educational events observed for this comparison of CMC modes
were the verbal elements in the content of dyad communications
that reflected cognitive or cooperative activities
which may represent a process of interpersonal cognitive
facilitation. Other sources of data were the assessments of the
outcomes of the cooperative task and the responses to questionnaires
and interviews revealing subjective impressions of the online
educational experience.
Although interpretation of the communication content was
essentially a qualitative assessment, it did involve counting
and categorizing of items which then took the form of numeric
values assigned to dependent variables. Similarly, the responses
to the questionnaires and the categorized responses to interview
questions were quantified. These values were then transformed
to descriptive statistics to facilitate the comparison of the two
modes of CMC interaction. In the context of educational CMC presented
in the previous paragraphs, the graphical and numeric presentation
of the findings at the descriptive level is sufficient for the
purposes of this study. They strongly suggest that there are
differences in the amount of cognitive and cooperative activity
that occurs in the two modes. They also suggest that the mode of
text-based CMC interaction affects the quality and correctness
of the outcomes for tasks such as the nursing diagnosis and nursing
care planning problem.
For studies in which testing of specific hypotheses is the
objective, inferential statistical procedures are used
to determine whether observed differences between groups are due
to the factors being investigated, or whether variation due to chance
or other factors is responsible. Since these procedures have strict
requirements for experimental control, independence of variables,
and assumptions of random sampling, they were neither appropriate
nor necessary for the warranting of claims in this study.
Nevertheless, questions invariably arise as to the relative strength
or degree of difference found in such comparisons. Also,
it seems to be accepted practice to apply and report inferential
statistics even when threats to validity are quite obvious. Perhaps
this is because the results of the statistical calculations provide
us with an enhanced description of the potential relationships
among factors even when inference is not strictly possible. For
example, graphical representation of the correlations in Figure
@Ref(fcorr1) on page @PageRef(fcorr1) provide one level of
representation, but the calculated strength of the relationship,
Pearson's r, increases the descriptive power of the findings.
Certainly, in a study such as this, with two groups
for comparison and a small sample size, the familiar t-test
can be applied to further our understanding of the differences
between the two groups. The t-test is used to enhance the
descriptive power of the data and findings, not to suggest
statistical significance nor to provide validity warrants.
Following Carver (1978), it is the full description of this research
@CiteMark(CarverRP78a)
process and its context that allows other researchers the
opportunity for replication or modification of the study.
Since some of the findings do show statistical significance,
the threats to validity must be addressed.
@Tag(limits)
First, the subjects for this study were not randomly
sampled from the total population. Rather, they represent a
convenience sample taken from one institution in one geographical
area. They were self-selected volunteers who were randomly
assigned to the comparison groups, not randomly sampled from
representative populations. Also, the coding of the transcripts
and the assessment of the nursing diagnosis and nursing care
planning outcomes were undertaken by one individual, the researcher.
Although two colleagues reviewed and verified the coding and
assessments of all the transcripts, there was no attempt to measure
inter-rater reliability. Further, the computer-based environments
themselves, using the CSILE software for asynchronous interaction and
the PHONE utility for synchronous interaction may not have provided
the best CMC capabilities for either mode. Although these environments
are comparable to those presently being used in the field, they
may have led to the comparison of a "worst-case" asynchronous mode
with a "best-case" synchronous mode, or vice versa.
Limitations concerning statistical significance can be found
in much of the reported research for studies with similar features
and contexts as this one. Even Powell (1986), from whom the content
@CiteMark(PowellJP86a)
categories for cognitive activity were drawn, cautioned readers about
the risks of inter-rater reliability using his system (p. 28). The
popular paper by Kiesler et al. (1984) describes experimental
@CiteMark(KieslerS84a)
designs and conditions, but does not report any values for statistical
significance. Dobos and Grieve (1985) in their study of the effects
@CiteMark(DobosJ85a)
of turn-taking protocols on decision productivity describe their
experience as follows: "A total of 16 groups (8 experimental, 8
control) were scheduled for the synchronous computer conferences.
System failure, however, caused cancellation of seven groups. Five
experimental (protocol) and four control (no protocol) groups
completed ..."(p. 377). Then, with cautions about their small cell
sizes (experimental N=18, control N=14), they report the results of
t-tests on the responses to a 26 item questionnaire concerning the
subjective impressions of the participants about their synchronous
conferencing experience. Three of these items showed "significant"
differences.
As noted at the beginning of this section, however, the
significance and importance of the present study (and those
mentioned in the previous paragraph) are not based on the
statistical significance of the findings. Rather, they are a
function of contextual and theoretical factors that guide the
research and project its relevance. The contexts and theories
which provided the focus for this study have been presented.
It can be shown, as well, that within this general area of research
there are studies that have similar characteristics. For example,
in terms of data generation factors, the investigation of
interactive written
discourse (IWD) by Ferrara et al. (1991) shows similarities in
the number of subjects (23), time online (2 hours), and number of
words per subject (712).
These factors of context, theory, and the general
representativeness of the current study, along with the specification
and elaboration of its methods and outcomes, provide the necessary
basis for validity and warranting of the claims.
With due consideration to the limitations stated on page @PageRef(limits),
four knowledge claims arise from the findings of this study.
The first is that task oriented discussions
in the synchronous mode of text-based computer-mediated communication
are more likely to include verbal elements reflecting important
cognitive activities such as problem formulation, interactive
arguing, and task management than similar discussions in asynchronous
mode. Second, the more highly rated outcomes from dyads using the
synchronous mode demonstrate greater adherence to solution protocols,
involving guidelines and principles, which improved the focus and
accuracy of their discussions relative to the task. As shown in
Figure @Ref(fcorr1) the key cognitive activities and the adherence
to principles and guidelines are well correlated with overall
ratings of nursing diagnoses and nursing care plans.
The third knowledge claim is that greater mutual
facilitation occurs in synchronous text-based CMC than in the
asynchronous mode. This facilitation is reflected in verbal
elements demonstrating attempts to establish interpersonal ease,
support, understanding, and encouragement. Cooperative activity
of this sort, however, was not well correlated with outcome
ratings (see Figure @Ref The most obvious theoretical implication arising from these
claims is that the role and application of synchronous text-based
computer-mediated communication in education is at least as important
as its asynchronous counterpart. The asynchronous mode has been
presented as one of the key characteristics of CMC for group support
and online educational applications@Cite(RapaportM91a,
HarasimLM90b, HiltzSR90a, TuroffM90a).
Not only does it provide the convenience
of time independence, but it is also reported to foster richer
intellectual exchanges@Cite(LevinsonP88a ", p. 115") and to
allow variation in the individual cognitive styles of
participants@Cite(TuroffM89b ", p. 11"). Synchronous CMC, on the
other hand, has been the
subject of various studies@Cite(DobosJ85a, SiegelJ86a, WiltonJA88a,
PeytonJK88a, WilkinsH91a), but synchronicity, per se, has not been
the focal issue.
The importance of synchronous interaction is revealed in
this study because the problem context (outlined previously in
Sections @Ref The importance of ICF in
cooperative learning theory and related theories of the social
construction of knowledge is also implicated in the findings of
this study. ICF, as measured by cognitive verbal
elements in the content of the peer discussions, was correlated
with improved task performance and outcomes. As such, the
measurement and understanding of learning in cooperative discussion
groups may well benefit from further elaboration and testing
of ICF processes. It should also be noted that cooperative
verbal elements did not correlate with task performance and
outcomes. This suggests that cognitive factors in the social
construction of knowledge (ICF in the ZPD, for example) may deserve
more attention than strictly socio-affective factors.
The implication of this research for the "linked learners"
scenario presented in the introduction is that we now have
further evidence to support the interdependent learning potential
of the model. The pro-active view of ICF is that of a
communication technique that can be actively promoted and
taught to members of the online learning community. Interpersonal
cognitive facilitation becomes more than a construct through
which learning activities can be measured and understood in peer
discussion groups. It becomes a cooperative technique through
which learners can facilitate
the learning of others using intentional communication.
The implications for theory in nursing education do not
derive specifically from the synchronicity issues of this
research. Neither is ICF implicated any further than has been
discussed previously. Rather, the responses to the questionnaire
and interview items by the nursing student participants concerning
their subjective impressions of the experience reveal an important
motivational effect from using the medium regardless of which
mode is used. Many students reported that the experience was good
because of the interaction with the views and approaches of another
person. They remarked on how interesting and positive it was to see
how other students worked out the problem: "just seeing their
thoughts". This enthusiasm was reported in the study by Sleightholm
Carins (1989) as well. It may not be too surprising if we consider
@CiteMark(SleightBJ89a)
how infrequently students get to see their classmates' work
in progress including their ways of understanding and resolving
various problems.
As data must be processed to make useful information and
information must be organized and understood to form knowledge,
knowledge must lead to action in order to be of value to human
existence. The findings and implications of this study, qualified
by reasonable validity constraints and formulated as an original
contribution to knowledge, provide grounds for further study into the
cognitive and cooperative verbal activities that occur in synchronous
versus asynchronous peer interaction. Further, they provide
sufficient basis for change in the practice of educational CMC.
The basic recommendations for change have been stated as value
claims in the Gowin's Vee outline of this
dissertation (see Section @Ref More generally, however, the feature that should be designed into
the software is the power and flexibility to access a whole host
of advanced tools for intellectual work. Features built
into the learning activities should be based on the nature
of the learning task, the desired outcomes, and an appropriate
instructional design philosophy. Technically, the facility for
synchronous
text-based interaction will have its role just as asynchronous
text-based interaction does. In the age of linked learners, however,
a broad spectrum of information utilities and knowledge tools must
be introduced to enable optimal levels of interactivity among humans
and machines. This interactivity will be a key element in advanced
computer supported cooperative learning environments.
5.2 Implications for Theory
5.3 Recommendations